homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Wolves can show as much attachment to humans as dogs do, hinting at how they became domesticated

Wolves were readily suited for domestication by early humans, the research shows.

Alexandru Micu
September 21, 2022 @ 9:01 pm

share Share

Wolves may share the ability of their close cousins, dogs, in attaching to human caregivers.

Image credits Andrea Bohl.

Dogs are man’s best friend, and a large part of their friendship is the level of attachment dogs can show to those who care for and live alongside them. According to new research, wolves also share this ability to show attachment behavior towards their human caregivers.

The most important implications of these findings are that they underscore how wolves — the wild ancestors of dogs — can readily form strong bonds to humans.

Friends of wolves

The paper adds to a body of accumulating evidence that contradicts the hypothesis that dogs’ ability to bond with humans was something bred into them during the domestication process.

“We felt that there was a need to thoroughly test this,” says Dr. Christina Hansen Wheat, PhD in Ethology from Stockholm University, Sweden and lead author of the paper. “Together with earlier studies making important contributions to this question, I think it is now appropriate to entertain the idea that if variation in human-directed attachment behaviour exists in wolves, this behaviour could have been a potential target for early selective pressures exerted during dog domestication.”

The findings are based on a study with 10 European grey wolves and 12 Alaskan husky dogs, which aimed to quantify and compare the level of attachment behaviours in these species. The wolves in this study were around 5 months (23 weeks) old at the time of the experiment.

Each of the animals was put through a discrimination task. This consisted of alternating between the presence and absence of a stranger and/or a familiar person in the test room with the animal. The familiar person was the primary caregiver for all of the pups, a woman that had raised and spent the most time with the animals since they were 10 days of age. The stranger was another woman, but who had never met neither the wolves nor the huskies.

The experimental room measured 6 by 6 meters (19.5 sq ft) with two chairs placed 2m away from each other in the middle of the room facing the same direction. Seven toys such as balls, rope, and rubber animals were brought into and distributed across the room from the home enclosures of the animals — familiar toys were used in order to avoid influencing the experiment’s outcome. Two diagonally-mounted GoPro cameras were used to record each animal’s reaction during the experiment.

“A total of seven behaviors were quantified […]. These seven behaviors included: (1) greeting, following, physical contact, and standing by the door — all categorized as safe haven effects, which are expressed as a means to maintain proximity or physical contact with the attachment figure; (2) exploration and play — both categorized as secure base effects, which can be expressed more in the presence of the attachment figure; and (3) passive behavior — categorized as other behavior related to other aspects of the social and physical environment,” the study explains.

Boiled down, what the experiment set out to determine was if wolves and dogs could discriminate between strangers and familiar people when in a strange situation. They did this by checking if the animals would spend more time greeting and in physical contact with the familiar person and show them more affection than they would a stranger.

All in all, the wolves spontaneously discriminated between the familiar and unfamiliar person similar to the dogs, showing more proximity-seeking and affiliative behaviors toward the familiar individual. Furthermore, both groups benefited from the presence of the familiar person, who acted as a buffer against social stress — in other words, both the wolves and the dogs were calmed by the presence of the familiar person during stressful situations. This suggests that the ability to bond with humans is equally-shared among dogs and wolves, and wasn’t something bred specifically into dogs through domestication.

“It was very clear that the wolves, as the dogs, preferred the familiar person over the stranger. But what was perhaps even more interesting was that while the dogs were not particularly affected by the test situation, the wolves were,” says Dr. Hansen Wheat. “They were pacing the test room.”

“However, the remarkable thing was that when the familiar person, a hand-raiser that had been with the wolves all their lives, re-entered the test room the pacing behaviour stopped, indicating that the familiar person acted as a social stress buffer for the wolves. I do not believe that this has ever been shown to be the case for wolves before and this also complements the existence of a strong bond between the animals and the familiar person.”

Although this similarity between dogs and wolves can seem surprising, Dr. Wheat explains that it does make more sense looking back. Being able to attach to humans would make for a distinctive advantage in the early stages of domestication for any animal.

In other words, it’s not that dogs grew closer to humans as they were domesticated, but their ability to become close to us is why they were so readily domesticated in the first place.

The paper “Human-directed attachment behavior in wolves suggests standing ancestral variation for human–dog attachment bonds” has been published in the journal Ecology and Evolution.

share Share

What Happens When Russian and Ukrainian Soldiers Come Home?

Russian and Ukrainian soldiers will eventually largely lay down their arms, but as the Soviet Afghanistan War shows, returning from the frontlines causes its own issues.

Some people are just wired to like music more, study shows

Most people enjoy music to some extent. But while some get goosebumps from their favorite song, others don’t really feel that much. A part of that is based on our culture. But according to one study, about half of it is written in our genes. In one of the largest twin studies on musical pleasure […]

This Stinky Coastal Outpost Made Royal Dye For 500 Years

Archaeologists have uncovered a reeking, violet-stained factory where crushed sea snails once fueled the elite’s obsession with royal purple.

Researchers analyzed 10,000 studies and found cannabis could actually fight cancer

Scientists used AI to scan a huge number of papers and found cannabis gets a vote of confidence from science.

Scientists Found a Way to Turn Falling Rainwater Into Electricity

It looks like plumbing but acts like a battery.

AI Made Up a Science Term — Now It’s in 22 Papers

A mistranslated term and a scanning glitch birthed the bizarre phrase “vegetative electron microscopy”

Elon Musk could soon sell missile defense to the Pentagon like a Netflix subscription

In January, President Donald Trump signed an executive order declaring missile attacks the gravest threat to America. It was the official greenlight for one of the most ambitious military undertakings in recent history: the so-called “Golden Dome.” Now, just months later, Elon Musk’s SpaceX and two of its tech allies—Palantir and Anduril—have emerged as leading […]

She Can Smell Parkinson’s—Now Scientists Are Turning It Into a Skin Swab

A super-smeller's gift could lead to an early, non-invasive Parkinson's test.

This Caddisfly Discovered Microplastics in 1971—and We Just Noticed

Decades before microplastics made headlines, a caddisfly larva was already incorporating synthetic debris into its home.

Have scientists really found signs of alien life on K2-18b?

Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence. We're not quite there.