homehome Home chatchat Notifications


The other replication crisis: research that's less likely to be true is cited more

Studies that are "out there" tend to be picked up more, even when they're on shaky ground. Meanwhile, solid science is sometimes overlooked.

Mihai Andrei
May 27, 2021 @ 10:42 pm

share Share

Although science publishing is by far the best option we have for advancing our understanding of the world, the publishing system itself is far from perfect.

In the early 2010s, a new term became very hot in some fields of research: replication crisis. The problem researchers had discovered was that many scientific studies are difficult or impossible to replicate or reproduce. Because reproducing research is an essential pillar of research, this has grave consequences, and forced us to reconsider many of the things we took for granted, especially in the fields of medicine and psychology.

There’s much to be said about the replication crisis, but one particular aspect was brought up in a new study: how un-replicable studies are cited.

Citations can make or break someone’s scientific career; the more citations one study or author has, the more it is regarded as important and influential. But according to a new study, research that is less likely to be replicable is also more likely to be cited.

The problem is not new for experts. In fact, to some extent, researchers in various fields are already aware of this problem.

“We also know that experts can predict well which papers will be replicated,” write the authors Marta Serra-Garcia, assistant professor of economics and strategy at the Rady School, and Uri Gneezy, professor of behavioral economics also at the Rady School. “Given this prediction, we ask ‘why are non-replicable papers accepted for publication in the first place?'”

“Interesting” results

The problem, Serra-Garcia suspects, is that the review teams of academic journals face a trade-off. In order for a paper to get published, a study first needs to be peer-reviewed — edited by experts in the field. When a study is published on something that’s well-known and established, and has useful results but lacks the ‘wow’ factor, it will likely be edited very harshly. But reviewers are more likely to be lenient when the results are more “out there.”

The same thing happens in the media: studies that are striking or more interesting somehow are more likely to be picked up, although their validity may be a bit more questionable.

“Interesting or appealing findings are also covered more by media or shared on platforms like Twitter, generating a lot of attention, but that does not make them true,” Gneezy said.

Serra-Garcia and Gneezy analyzed data from three influential replication projects which tried to systematically replicate the findings in top psychology, economic, and general science journals like Nature and Science. In economics, 61% of 18 studies were successfully replicated; a similar figure was found for general science (62%). This is already less than ideal, but in psychology, things were way worse: just 39 of 100 experiments could successfully be replicated.

The disparity in citations is striking. Papers that are successfully replicated are cited 153 times less than those that couldn’t be replicated. Even when researchers took into account several characteristics of the replicated studies (the number of authors, the rate of male authors, the details of the experiment, and the field) — the relation between citations and replicability was still unchanged.

The impact of such citations also grows over time, becoming more pronounced as time passes. In other words, it’s not just a fad where some studies have unusual results and they get quoted at first but then researchers catch on — the effect continues over time and, on average, papers that could not be replicated are cited 16 times more per year.

“Remarkably, only 12 percent of post-replication citations of non-replicable findings acknowledge the replication failure,” the authors write.

An impactful problem

To see just how big a problem this is, you need look no further than the vaccine-autism controversy. It all started from a study published by Andrew Wakefield in 1998. The study has long been retracted, and Wakefield’s methods were shown to be not just fraudulent but also cruel to the participants — yet despite numerous studies disproving Wakefield’s study, claims that autism is linked to vaccines still continue.

The problem can be solved by improving the way scientific publishing works. Academics are under tremendous pressure to publish papers, especially groundbreaking papers that get a lot of citations. If unreliable papers are more likely to gather citations, then academics have an incentive to publish this type of study. Modern science is generally built on small, incremental progress, not big breakthrough leaps, but small incremental progress isn’t flashy.

The authors hope to raise attention to this problem, and encourage researchers (and readers) to think that something that is interesting and appealing may not always be replicable.

“We hope our research encourages readers to be cautious if they read something that is interesting and appealing,” Serra-Garcia said. “Whenever researchers cite work that is more interesting or has been cited a lot, we hope they will check if replication data is available and what those findings suggest.”

The study is published in Science.

share Share

A Dutch 17-Year-Old Forgot His Native Language After Knee Surgery and Spoke Only English Even Though He Had Never Used It Outside School

He experienced foreign language syndrome for about 24 hours, and remembered every single detail of the incident even after recovery.

Your Brain Hits a Metabolic Cliff at 43. Here’s What That Means

This is when brain aging quietly kicks in.

Scientists Just Found a Hidden Battery Life Killer and the Fix Is Shockingly Simple

A simple tweak could dramatically improve the lifespan of Li-ion batteries.

Westerners cheat AI agents while Japanese treat them with respect

Japan’s robots are redefining work, care, and education — with lessons for the world.

Scientists Turn to Smelly Frogs to Fight Superbugs: How Their Slime Might Be the Key to Our Next Antibiotics

Researchers engineer synthetic antibiotics from frog slime that kill deadly bacteria without harming humans.

This Popular Zero-Calorie Sugar Substitute May Be Making You Hungrier, Not Slimmer

Zero-calorie sweeteners might confuse the brain, especially in people with obesity

Any Kind of Exercise, At Any Age, Boosts Your Brain

Even light physical activity can sharpen memory and boost mood across all ages.

A Brain Implant Just Turned a Woman’s Thoughts Into Speech in Near Real Time

This tech restores speech in real time for people who can’t talk, using only brain signals.

Using screens in bed increases insomnia risk by 59% — but social media isn’t the worst offender

Forget blue light, the real reason screens disrupt sleep may be simpler than experts thought.

Beetles Conquered Earth by Evolving a Tiny Chemical Factory

There are around 66,000 species of rove beetles and one researcher proposes it's because of one special gland.