homehome Home chatchat Notifications


At age four, most children are willing to make sacrifices in the name of group loyalty

Loyalty is a complex emotion, and paradoxical at times. For many loyalty is intrinsically link to identity, since our loyalty and allegiance to our nationality, families and friends or lack thereof defines us as persons. Psychologists have been studying the interplay of social injustice, righteous anger and group allegiance of many years (marketers especially love these kinds of studies). Loyal persons are seen by others as nicer and more trustworthy. But how early in life do we actually see group loyalty develop? As early as age four, a new research suggests.

Tibi Puiu
December 10, 2015 @ 9:19 pm

share Share

Loyalty is a complex emotion, and paradoxical at times. For many loyalty is intrinsically link to identity, since our loyalty and allegiance to our nationality, families and friends or lack thereof defines us as persons. Psychologists have been studying the interplay of social injustice, righteous anger and group allegiance of many years (marketers especially love these kinds of studies). Loyal persons are seen by others as nicer and more trustworthy. But how early in life do we actually see group loyalty develop? As early as age four, a new research suggests.

loyalty

Image:Clipartpanda

Antonia Misch and colleagues from Yale’s Social Development Cognitive Lab enlisted  48 five-year-olds and 48 four-year-olds, each group with equal numbers of boys and girls. They then introduced each toddler to four cute hand-puppets. Later, yellow and green scarfs were pulled from a box which signified two different group allegiances. The child and two of the puppets were assigned as a team using one of the scarfs (yellow or green), while the rest of the two puppets were made to wear the other coloured scarf.

I won’t tell

puppets

Puppets used in the study. Figure from Misch et al

The child and researcher then had to leave the room, presumably to look for something. Upon their return, the two puppets from their team or — alternatively, depending on whether this scenario was in-group or out-group — two puppets wearing different scarfs, caught red-handed hiding a book. The hand-puppets told the child that he must not tell anyone about this. Mere minutes later, a fifth puppet wearing no scarf (no group affiliation) introduced itself telling the child that it knew there was a secret kept in this room. The puppet offered to bribe the child with a sticker.

Most children resisted the temptation, 61% overall whether the puppets keeping the secret were from their team or not. Key, however, was that the kids who were urged to keep the secret by puppets wearing the same scarf were much likelier to stay steadfast in front of the bribing neutral puppet, as opposed to when asked by the other team (75% vs 48%). In-group loyalty was distinctly present in both age groups, but more pronounced in five year olds; 71 per cent of five-year-olds kept the secret compared with 52 per cent of four-year-olds.

Of note is that these in-group loyalties form very, very fast. The children were introduced to their team puppets and  allegiance in a process that took just a couple of minutes. The researchers expect that their real life in-group loyalties (family, peers) are much stronger.

Findings appeared in the Journal of experimental child psychology.

share Share

This 5,500-year-old Kish tablet is the oldest written document

Beer, goats, and grains: here's what the oldest document reveals.

A Huge, Lazy Black Hole Is Redefining the Early Universe

Astronomers using the James Webb Space Telescope have discovered a massive, dormant black hole from just 800 million years after the Big Bang.

Did Columbus Bring Syphilis to Europe? Ancient DNA Suggests So

A new study pinpoints the origin of the STD to South America.

The Magnetic North Pole Has Shifted Again. Here’s Why It Matters

The magnetic North pole is now closer to Siberia than it is to Canada, and scientists aren't sure why.

For better or worse, machine learning is shaping biology research

Machine learning tools can increase the pace of biology research and open the door to new research questions, but the benefits don’t come without risks.

This Babylonian Student's 4,000-Year-Old Math Blunder Is Still Relatable Today

More than memorializing a math mistake, stone tablets show just how advanced the Babylonians were in their time.

Sixty Years Ago, We Nearly Wiped Out Bed Bugs. Then, They Started Changing

Driven to the brink of extinction, bed bugs adapted—and now pesticides are almost useless against them.

LG’s $60,000 Transparent TV Is So Luxe It’s Practically Invisible

This TV screen vanishes at the push of a button.

Couple Finds Giant Teeth in Backyard Belonging to 13,000-year-old Mastodon

A New York couple stumble upon an ancient mastodon fossil beneath their lawn.

Worms and Dogs Thrive in Chernobyl’s Radioactive Zone — and Scientists are Intrigued

In the Chernobyl Exclusion Zone, worms show no genetic damage despite living in highly radioactive soil, and free-ranging dogs persist despite contamination.