homehome Home chatchat Notifications


The heartbreaking reason why you shouldn't yell at your dog

Dogs really suffer when you scream or punish them, and it affects their mental health.

Mihai Andrei
February 24, 2020 @ 8:33 pm

share Share

Yelling and punishing dogs makes them sadder and more pessimistic.

Ooph. Image credits: Matthew Henry.

As anyone who’s had a dog can attest, they are a source of immense joy and happiness. Dogs make people happier and they also make them healthier, new studies have shown. But people want dogs to behave — and lose their temper when this isn’t the case.

But even if doggies can be mischievous at times, yelling and punishment should not be the answer

Punishment breeds pessimism

It’s been suggested for quite a while that negative reinforcement can pose quite a strain on dogs — yet this is still what many owners and training schools opt for. To see whether punishment really does have negative consequences on dogs’ mental health, a team of researchers recruited 92 companion dogs from three reward-based and four punishment-based training schools (also called aversive-based).

The researchers, led by biologist Ana Catarina Vieira de Castro of the Universidade do Porto in Portugal, carried out both short-term and long-term welfare assessments for dogs in both groups.

For the short-term assessments, the dogs were recorded over three training sessions, and saliva samples were gathered to analyze their cortisol levels (cortisol being well-known as an indicator of high stress). Video recordings were also made after the training sessions to allow the researchers to examine the frequency of stress-related behaviors (such as lip licks or yawns).

For the long-term assessment, dogs performed a cognitive bias task — a task that would reveal patterns of deviation in judgment, representative of the animals’ emotional states.

Assessing pessimism in animals is not a novel practice. In this case, the dogs were trained to associate a bowl on one side of a room with a sausage snack. If the bowl was on one side, it always had a treat. If it was on the other side, it never had a treat.

Both bowls were rubbed with sausage to prevent the smell from giving it off.

Then, the bows were moved around the room to ambiguous locations, to see how quickly the dogs would appreciate the bowl, hoping for a treat. The quicker they’d go, the more optimistic they are. If they took their time, it mean that the dog was more pessimistic.

Sure enough, the more aversive training dogs had, the more time they took before approaching the bowl.

No yelling, please

The branch manager deserves love and care, not punishment. Image credits: Jamie Street.

Dogs from the aversive-based schools exhibited more stress-related behavior and exhibited an overall lower mood than their counterparts — which was also indicated by their high cortisol levels.

In the long term, dogs who were yelled at or punished during training were also more pessimistic.

“Our results show that companion dogs trained using aversive-based methods experienced poorer welfare as compared to companion dogs trained using reward-based methods, at both the short- and the long-term level,” the researchers write in the new paper, which was published on the pre-print server bioRxiv.

“Specifically, dogs attending schools using aversive-based methods displayed more stress-related behaviours and body postures during training, higher elevations in cortisol levels after training, and were more ‘pessimistic’ in a cognitive bias task.”

These findings indicate that the use of aversive-based methods threatens the emotional wellbeing of dogs both short-term and long term.

By contrast, the reward-based training schools seemed to cause no major shift in behavior — so it’s not training dogs that affects them, it’s the punishment-based training.

It’s not the first time something like this has been suggested. A 2017 literature review of studies reported a similar effect, but noted that most studies are based on surveys (not objective measurements) and are mostly focused on police and laboratory dogs.

Now, this study expands the findings to a range of companion dogs. The bottom line — training or no training, dogs shouldn’t be yelled at or punished, they should be loved and cherish (this was not a part of the study, but it goes without saying).

You can read the study here.

share Share

Same-Sex Behavior Is Surprisingly Common in Animals — Humans Are No Exception

Some people claim same-sex attraction is "unnatural." Biology says otherwise

Crows seem to understand geometry — and we thought only humans could

In a remarkable new study, crows demonstrated an intuitive grasp of geometry—identifying irregular shapes without training.

In 2013, dolphins in Florida starved. Now, we know why

The culprit is a very familiar one. It's us.

Could man's best friend be an environmental foe?

Even good boys and girls can disrupt wildlife in ways you never expected.

Alcohol Helps Male Fruit Flies Get Lucky—But They Know When to Stop

Male fruit flies use booze to boost pheromones and charm potential mates—just not too much.

But they're not really dire wolves, are they?

and this isn't a conservation story

A 97-Year-Old Tortoise Just Became a First-Time Mom at the Philadelphia Zoo

Mommy has been living at the Philadelphia Zoo for 90 years, and waited until old age to experience motherhood.

Birds Are Changing Color in Cities. Here’s Why

Birds in cities are getting flashier — literally.

Gardening Really Is Good for You, Science Confirms

Gardening might do more for your health than you think.

Underwater Tool Use: These Rainbow-Colored Fish Smash Shells With Rocks

Wrasse fish crack open shells with rocks in behavior once thought exclusive to mammals and birds.