homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Despite their amazing achievements, leaf-cutter ants don't coordinate work

Work hard, not smart!

Alexandru Micu
January 24, 2019 @ 3:43 pm

share Share

There’s no work quite like teamwork — unless you’re a leaf-cutter ant.


Photo by Scott Bauer, public domain.

“Leaf-cutter ant” is a catch-many term referring to some 47 species of ants in 2 genera, Atta and Acromyrex, found in Central and South America. They all share the same way of life, feeding on fungi/mushrooms that they themselves grow in their mounds.

But agriculture is hard work, and involves proper fertilization of crops to reap the full rewards. The ants know this, and much of their day is spent on cutting leaves (uncanny, I know), flowers, and other types of plant matter to use as a substrate for their ‘fields’. Another big part of the mound’s operations, therefore, involves the construction and proper maintenance of infrastructure — ‘superhighways’ hundreds of meters long — that allow easy access to and from the raw resources these ants require.

Leaf-cutter ants form some of the largest and most complex communities on Earth, perhaps second only to our own. Factor in the scale of their infrastructure and how seamlessly it’s built, and we’ve just assumed that there must be some level of organization among the ants. New research shows, however, that this isn’t the case.

Public works

“Although many thousand individuals contribute to the construction of infrastructure, there is no communication or organisation between them -— the massive foraging trails are a byproduct of the foraging behaviour,” said lead-author Thomas Bochynek from Northwestern University’s department of electrical engineering and computer science.

Leaf-cutter colonies can build nearly three kilometers of trails per year, and invest an average of 11,000 work-hours into constructing and maintaining them. Common wisdom held that such projects are handled through stigmergy — delegating tasks to specialists via direct or indirect communication among individuals. They were believed to leave behind pheromone trails to tell their fellow colony members where extra help is needed in removing a particularly heavy twig, for example.

Bochynek’s team, however, show that the ants handle their infrastructure projects without any coordination whatsoever. Each ant seems to act individually, they write, handling tasks as they are encountered.

Errors may happen, though.
Image credits Geoff Gallice / Wikimedia.

The team monitored colonies of leaf-cutter ants in the wild and in their lab to see how fast they could carve a path through debris and how many individuals would be assigned to the task. They also used computer models to establish the randomized rate of clearance based on each ant’s likelihood of encountering an obstruction by chance and then clearing it.

In essence, this last step gave the team a baseline value for how fast a colony could deal with new tasks. If the ants spent time communicating and organizing to clear a particular spot, the rate would temporarily go down. Once they were all set and got to work, this rate would go up.

Experimental obstruction arena (100 × 50 cm) the team set up in their lab. The trail was blocked with folded blue paper strips (dotted lines, seen magnified in the bottom left). The obstruction arena was placed between the nest (to the left) and the foraging container (to the right).
Image credits Thomas Bochynek et al., (2019), PRSB.

What the team found, however, was an almost linear increase in the number of obstacles removed over time. This didn’t fit the hypothesis that the ants were coordinating. Instead, it suggested that ants went out and handled problems as they encountered them, taking no downtime to communicate and organize. The team believes ants evolved into this particular work ethic because it helps reduce overall energy expenditure for the colony (which means it can keep going on less food) and doesn’t require particular mental oomph from the ants.

“If you do not need to communicate, don’t!”, says Bochynek. “It saves the energetic expenditure of performing the behaviour and reduces the required complexity in the individual animals.”

Needless to say, this is distinctively un-human behavior — but that’s what makes it interesting. Bochynek hopes his team’s results will help spur further research into how insects maximize energy efficiency and handle large-scale projects without oversight.

“Biologists and computer scientists are impressed by the fact that complexity in a group can come from simple individuals because it is so different from our own society,” he said.

“Showing that we can further reduce the minimum amount of complexity required in an individual to still see complex group behaviour is hence exciting, and makes the trail clearing example more impressive in our opinion.”

One area where this research might be particularly interesting is robotics — particularly, robotic swarms and robot cooperation. Bochynek’s study shows that individual complexity is not required for a group to exhibit complex behavior. In other words, that even pretty ‘dumb’ robots working together can perform complex tasks with minimal oversight. Now we just need to write the code.

The paper “Infrastructure construction without information exchange: the trail clearing mechanism in Atta leafcutter ants” has been published in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

share Share

Evolution just keeps creating the same deep-ocean mutation

Creatures at the bottom of the ocean evolve the same mutation — and carry the scars of human pollution

Scientists Found a 380-Million-Year-Old Trick in Velvet Worm Slime That Could Lead To Recyclable Bioplastic

Velvet worm slime could offer a solution to our plastic waste problem.

Beetles Conquered Earth by Evolving a Tiny Chemical Factory

There are around 66,000 species of rove beetles and one researcher proposes it's because of one special gland.

These researchers counted the trees in China using lasers

The answer is 142 billion. Plus or minus a few, of course.

New Diagnostic Breakthrough Identifies Bacteria With Almost 100% Precision in Hours, Not Days

A new method identifies deadly pathogens with nearly perfect accuracy in just three hours.

This Tamagotchi Vape Dies If You Don’t Keep Puffing

Yes. You read that correctly. The Stupid Hackathon is an event like no other.

Wild Chimps Build Flexible Tools with Impressive Engineering Skills

Chimpanzees select and engineer tools with surprising mechanical precision to extract termites.

Archaeologists in Egypt discovered a 3,600-Year-Old pharaoh. But we have no idea who he is

An ancient royal tomb deep beneath the Egyptian desert reveals more questions than answers.

Researchers create a new type of "time crystal" inside a diamond

“It’s an entirely new phase of matter.”

Strong Arguments Matter More Than Grammar in English Essays as a Second Language

Grammar takes a backseat to argumentation, a new study from Japan suggests.