homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Moles smell in stereo to navigate for food

Stereo sensing is a highly important skill that most animals possess that creates a directional perspective. Not all senses are stereo, however, in some animals. For instance, humans have stereo vision and hearing, however no stereo smell – the latter being a trait that not too many animals possess. The common mole has been found […]

Tibi Puiu
February 6, 2013 @ 9:08 am

share Share

The eastern mole (Scalopus aquaticus) has its nostrils very closely separated, however it's been proven to be the first mammal to posses stereo smell. (c) Kenneth Catania, Wikipedia Commons

The eastern mole (Scalopus aquaticus) has its nostrils very closely separated, however it’s been proven to be the first mammal to posses stereo smell. (c) Kenneth Catania, Wikipedia Commons

Stereo sensing is a highly important skill that most animals possess that creates a directional perspective. Not all senses are stereo, however, in some animals. For instance, humans have stereo vision and hearing, however no stereo smell – the latter being a trait that not too many animals possess. The common mole has been found to display such an ability, after researchers at Vanderbilt University found the mammal relies on stereo smell to locate prey.

Few animals have so far been proven to have stereo smell, like some species of ants and sharks, although some that heavily rely on their sense of smell like pigs, mice or dogs might also possess this skill. The common mole (Scalopus aquaticus), which can be found wrecking havoc through backyard gardens anywhere from eastern United States and Canada to Mexico, is now the first mammal proven to smell in stereo.

“I came at this as a skeptic,” neuroscientist Kenneth Catania of Vanderbilt University was quoted as saying in a press release. “I thought the moles’ nostrils were too close together to effectively detect odor gradients. The fact that moles use stereo odor cues to locate food suggests other mammals that rely heavily on their sense of smell, like dogs and pigs, might also have this ability.”

The human auditory system is able to discriminate whether a sound comes from the right or from the left, if it is located behind, in front of, below or above the hearer. As a matter of fact, humans can distinguish sounds from two different sources, where the angle between the sources is only 3° – that’s very accurate. Similarly, with stereo smell one can detect different odor sources from each sources, with the brain being also wired to detect these differences. A sense of depth and orientation all based on smell is thus possible – for the common mole, at least.

Catania’s inspiration to study the common mole can be tracked back as far as ten years ago when he studied another mole, the star-nosed mole which uses a sort of tentacle-like tissue on its nose to sense food by touch.  He decided to test the common moles’ capability to find prey for comparison purposes.

“I expected the common mole, which is virtually blind and doesn’t have a very good sense of touch, to be a lot worse than the star-nosed mole. So I was quite surprised when they turned out to be very good at locating prey. At the time, I figured that they must be using their sense of smell, but I didn’t pursue the matter.”

[Also read] White smell: the neutral fragrance discovered by scientists

Since the two performed the same at finding food, despite sensing discrepancies between the two species, Catania naturally reasoned that the common mole’s sense of smell must be more sophisticated that previously thought. Catania presumed the mole must have stereo smell in order to guide itself and find mole, considering all its other senses didn’t provide navigation, so he put his idea to the test.

Where’s that smell coming from?

sniff He made a radial arena with food wells spaced around a 180-degree circle with the entrance for the mole located at the center. Multiple tests were carried out with food (earthworms) placed randomly at different locations, while the arena was sealed at all time in order for Catania to see every time the mole sniffed by measuring the difference in air pressure. When the mole was introduced, Catania observed how the mammal first sniffed a bit back and forth around its location then immediately zeroed in on its target, the food source, on a direct path.

“It was amazing,” he said. ”They found the food in less than five seconds and went to the right food well almost every time. They have a hyper-sensitive sense of smell.”

To see whether indeed stereo smell aided navigation, Catania blocked each of mole’s two nostrils at a time and repeated the test. When the left nostril was blocked, the mole would still be able to locate food, but its path towards it was veered to left. The same, only opposite, happened when the right nostril was blocked.

“This is strikingly similar to a landmark study of hearing in barn owls performed in 1979 byEric Knudsen and Mark Konishi at the California Institute of Technology, who found that blocking one of the owl’s ears caused them to misjudge the location of a sound source,” Catania said.

In the ultimate test that finally proved moles indeed posses stereo smell Catania reversed the mole’s nostrils  after he inserted plastic tubes in both nostrils that were reversed. Thus, the right nostril was sniffing air on the animal’s left and the left nostril was sniffing air on the animal’s right. When their nostrils were crossed in this fashion, the animals searched back and forth and frequently could not find the food at all.

Findings were published in the journal Nature Communications. [source]

ZME readers, is the mole cool or what? Discuss in the comments section below. 

share Share

For better or worse, machine learning is shaping biology research

Machine learning tools can increase the pace of biology research and open the door to new research questions, but the benefits don’t come without risks.

Sixty Years Ago, We Nearly Wiped Out Bed Bugs. Then, They Started Changing

Driven to the brink of extinction, bed bugs adapted—and now pesticides are almost useless against them.

Worms and Dogs Thrive in Chernobyl’s Radioactive Zone — and Scientists are Intrigued

In the Chernobyl Exclusion Zone, worms show no genetic damage despite living in highly radioactive soil, and free-ranging dogs persist despite contamination.

These "Ants" Use Ultrablack to Warn Predators — and Stay Cool

Velvet ants, actually flightless wasps, boast an ultrablack exoskeleton thanks to dense nanostructures.

These Squirrels Are Hunting and Eating Meat. Scientists Are Stunned — And They Have Video Proof

California ground squirrels surprise scientists with their newly discovered taste for mammalian flesh.

Scientists Call for a Global Pause on Creating “Mirror Life” Before It’s Too Late: “The threat we’re talking about is unprecedented”

Creating synthetic lifeforms is almost here, and the consequences could be devastating.

This Hornet Can Drink 80% Alcohol Without Ever Getting Drunk and Scientists Finally Know Why

Oriental hornets never get intoxicated with alcohol no matter how strong the alcohol or how long they drink.

This Tiny Microbe Can Withstand Extreme Radiation That Would Obliterate Humans. Here's How It Might Protect Astronauts on a Trip to Mars

Could a humble bacterium hold the key to surviving cosmic radiation?

The heart may have its own "mini-brain": a nervous system that controls heartbeat

Somewhere within the heart, there may be a "little brain".

Crocodile Scales Form in a Surprising Way That Has Nothing to Do with Genetics

The surprising way crocodile scales form offers a glimpse into how evolution works beyond genes.