homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Human brain perceives men as persons and women as parts, study finds

When you first see this magnificent painting by Ukranian painter Oleg Shuplyak, your brain perceives the portrait of the famous Beatles frontman John Lenon. However, on a closer look, one will immediately notice that the portrait is actually made out of a sum of parts – a table and a troubadour make for the mouth, two men […]

Tibi Puiu
July 25, 2012 @ 4:45 pm

share Share

Oleg Schuplyek Imagine When you first see this magnificent painting by Ukranian painter Oleg Shuplyak, your brain perceives the portrait of the famous Beatles frontman John Lenon. However, on a closer look, one will immediately notice that the portrait is actually made out of a sum of parts – a table and a troubadour make for the mouth, two men dressed in long clothes make for the eyes and cheeks, a building’s double archway makes for the eyebrows and so on. There are two mental functions which help us distinguish shapes and geometry into objects – global when the sum is put ahead of the parts, and local when the parts are put ahead of the sum.

In a study which tackles a highly controversial subject, psychologists at University of  Nebraska-Lincoln have found that people, men and women included, perceived images of men as whole (global cognition), while images of women were processed as an assemblage of its various parts (local cognition). The researchers believe this is a deeply rooted human cognition feature, which serves to  provide clues as to why women are often the targets of sexual objectification. I told you this was controversial.

“Local processing underlies the way we think about objects: houses, cars and so on. But global processing should prevent us from that when it comes to people,” said Sarah Gervais, assistant professor of psychology at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln and the study’s lead autho. “We don’t break people down to their parts – except when it comes to women, which is really striking. Women were perceived in the same ways that objects are viewed.”

The participants were presented in a random manner with  dozens of images of fully clothed, average-looking men and women. Each person was shown from head to knee, standing, with eyes focused on the camera. For each individual, the system paused, after which it presented the participant with two images, slightly different from one another, from which the participant had to choose quickly what he  or she thought was the original. One of the images had a slightly modified version of the original image that comprised a sexual body part.

Participants saw a fully clothed person from head to knee. After a brief pause, they then saw two new images on their screen: One that was unmodified and contained the original image, the other a slightly modified version of the original image with a sexual body part changed. Participants then quickly indicated which of the two images they had previously seen. They made decisions about entire bodies in some trials and body parts in other trials. (c) University of Nebraska-Lincoln

Participants saw a fully clothed person from head to knee. After a brief pause, they then saw two new images on their screen: One that was unmodified and contained the original image, the other a slightly modified version of the original image with a sexual body part changed. Participants then quickly indicated which of the two images they had previously seen. They made decisions about entire bodies in some trials and body parts in other trials. (c) University of Nebraska-Lincoln

Women’s sexual body parts were more easily recognized when presented in isolation than when they were presented in the context of their entire bodies. But men’s sexual body parts were recognized better when presented in the context of their entire bodies than they were in isolation.

“We always hear that women are reduced to their sexual body parts; you hear about examples in the media all the time. This research takes it a step further and finds that this perception spills over to everyday women, too,” Gervais said. “The subjects in the study’s images were everyday, ordinary men and women … the fact that people are looking at ordinary men and women and remembering women’s body parts better than their entire bodies was very interesting.”

If you thought it was the men solely that perceived women in such a manner, you’d be wrong. Both men and women come out with more or less the same results.

“It’s both men and women doing this to women,” Gervais said. “So don’t blame the men here.”

In a second experiment the same participants were presented with images of letters made up of other tiny letters  — an H made up of hundreds of little Ts, for example. Some were asked to identify the bigger, whole letters to test their global cognition, while other were asked to perceive the smaller letters that made up the big letter to test their local cognition. Participants involved in the global processing part of the test were less likely to objectify women, the researchers found. They no longer were better at recognizing a woman’s parts than her whole body.

The second test proves that objectifying women is a habit that came be overcome, suggest the scientists involved in the study.

“Our findings suggest people fundamentally process women and men differently, but we are also showing that a very simple manipulation counteracts this effect, and perceivers can be prompted to see women globally, just as they do men,” Gervais said. “Based on these findings, there are several new avenues to explore.

Findings were published in the  European Journal of Social Psychology.

source

share Share

A Dutch 17-Year-Old Forgot His Native Language After Knee Surgery and Spoke Only English Even Though He Had Never Used It Outside School

He experienced foreign language syndrome for about 24 hours, and remembered every single detail of the incident even after recovery.

Looks Matter Most on Dating Apps By Far. And That's True for Both Men and Women

If you're struggling on dating apps, it's not because of your bio.

Just 10 Minutes of Mindfulness a Day Can Boost Your Mental Health

Daily short mindfulness sessions significantly reduce depression and anxiety while encouraging healthier lifestyles.

Why Can't We Remember Our Lives as Babies? Our Earliest Memories May Still be There

New research suggests infants can form memories far earlier than previously thought, but where do they go?

Less Than 1% of Gun Owners Use Their Firearms for Self-Defense Each Year. But Many More Are Exposed to Gun Violence

The study suggests gun proliferation carries more risks than self-defense benefits.

People Tend to Assume Heroes Like Spider-Man Would Vote for Their Party—And Villains Like Darth Vader Would for the Opposide Side

There's a strong tendancy to see political rivals as villains -- and this is a problem for democracyh.

Why Trying to Be Happy Ironically Makes You Unhappy

Chasing happiness may drain your mental energy, making you less happy in the long run.

People Who Blocked Their Smartphone's Internet for Two Weeks Report Big Mood Boost

Digital detox seems to work wonders.

AI Is Supposed to Make You More Productive — It's Making You Dumber and Overconfident

Generative AI is supposed to make life easier. It drafts emails, summarizes documents, and even generates creative content, helping you offload some of that dreaded cognitive effort. But according to a new study from Carnegie Mellon University and Microsoft Research, it may also be making you dumber in the process. The study, based on a […]

From the vault: Why bats don't fly in the rain

Ever wondered why you never see bats flying in the rain?