homehome Home chatchat Notifications


What your pupil says about your language

A simple word is enough to trigger a reaction in your pupil.

Mihai Andrei
June 20, 2017 @ 8:10 pm

share Share

A simple word is enough to trigger a reaction in your pupil.

The pupillary dilation. Image credits: Greyson Orlando.

A surprising new study found that when we hear words associated with a strong luminosity (ie “sun” or “shine”) our pupils contract as if we were actually exposed to them. The same thing happens to words we associate with darkness — our pupils dilate. The responses can have a variety of causes, from an involuntary reflex reaction to feelings of arousal to exposure to light. The latter is most common and often most pronounced.

This mechanism is done through the optic and oculomotor cranial nerve. Many creatures, humans included, exhibit a pupillary response. This is basically a mechanism through which the brain tries to adapt the body to new conditions, but it’s not totally clear why the reaction is also associated with psychological responses. What this study did is to open new avenues of research, and show that the dilation and contraction mechanism might be more complex than we thought.

“Theories about embodiment of language hold that when you process a word’s meaning, you automatically simulate associated sensory input (e.g., perception of brightness when you process lamp) and prepare associated actions (e.g., finger movements when you process typing),” the study reads. “To test this latter prediction, we measured pupillary responses to single words that conveyed a sense of brightness (e.g., day) or darkness (e.g., night) or were neutral (e.g., house).”

When confronted with a word, the pupils begin by dilating (0 — 0.5 s), following the general activation of the brain. When this initial activation has passed, the pupils retract (0.5 — 2 s). But the size of the pupil is also determined by the luminosity evoked by the words: when we read a luminance-associated word, the pupils become smaller than when we read a word associated with darkness (1 — 3 s). Image credits: Sebastiaan Mathot, University of Groningen.

Not all responses were alike. The brighter (or darker) the word people heard, the stronger the response, which in itself seems to raise more questions than it answers.

It seems to fit with a theory called the ’embodiment of language’. Basically, the theory says that whenever we hear a word or a group of words, we mentally simulate it in our mind. If someone would say ‘keyboard,’ your brain would project an image of the keyboard, as well as the gesture of typing at a keyboard; even if you might not realize it. The same thing happens with ‘sun’ — you visualize a big ball of fire, and your pupil adapts. However, researchers say, behavioral studies have so far not directly tested one of the central predictions of embodied language: that word meaning by itself can trigger, at least in some cases, associated involuntary actions. This is why this particular study is so important: it can be a definite proof for a long-standing but still challenged theory.

Journal Reference: Sebastiaan Mathôt, Jonathan Grainger, Kristof Strijkers. Pupillary Responses to Words That Convey a Sense of Brightness or Darkness. Psychological Science, 2017; 095679761770269 DOI: 10.1177/0956797617702699

 

share Share

This 5,500-year-old Kish tablet is the oldest written document

Beer, goats, and grains: here's what the oldest document reveals.

A Huge, Lazy Black Hole Is Redefining the Early Universe

Astronomers using the James Webb Space Telescope have discovered a massive, dormant black hole from just 800 million years after the Big Bang.

Did Columbus Bring Syphilis to Europe? Ancient DNA Suggests So

A new study pinpoints the origin of the STD to South America.

The Magnetic North Pole Has Shifted Again. Here’s Why It Matters

The magnetic North pole is now closer to Siberia than it is to Canada, and scientists aren't sure why.

For better or worse, machine learning is shaping biology research

Machine learning tools can increase the pace of biology research and open the door to new research questions, but the benefits don’t come without risks.

This Babylonian Student's 4,000-Year-Old Math Blunder Is Still Relatable Today

More than memorializing a math mistake, stone tablets show just how advanced the Babylonians were in their time.

Sixty Years Ago, We Nearly Wiped Out Bed Bugs. Then, They Started Changing

Driven to the brink of extinction, bed bugs adapted—and now pesticides are almost useless against them.

LG’s $60,000 Transparent TV Is So Luxe It’s Practically Invisible

This TV screen vanishes at the push of a button.

Couple Finds Giant Teeth in Backyard Belonging to 13,000-year-old Mastodon

A New York couple stumble upon an ancient mastodon fossil beneath their lawn.

Worms and Dogs Thrive in Chernobyl’s Radioactive Zone — and Scientists are Intrigued

In the Chernobyl Exclusion Zone, worms show no genetic damage despite living in highly radioactive soil, and free-ranging dogs persist despite contamination.