homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Babies can tell two languages apart as early as seven months-old

A new study by scientists at University of British Columbia and Université Paris Descartes found that babies growing up in bilingual environments are more than well equipped to tackle the challenge of distinguishing between the two from a very early age – as young as seven months old according to the findings. Scientists, linguistics and […]

Tibi Puiu
February 19, 2013 @ 10:55 am

share Share

A new study by scientists at University of British Columbia and Université Paris Descartes found that babies growing up in bilingual environments are more than well equipped to tackle the challenge of distinguishing between the two from a very early age – as young as seven months old according to the findings.

Baby Leif (centre, pictured with parents Harumi Inokoshi and Eric Roe) is growing up bilingual, hearing Japanese from his mother and English from his father.  (Photo: Janet Werker, UBC Infant Studies Centre)

Baby Leif (centre, pictured with parents Harumi Inokoshi and Eric Roe) is growing up bilingual, hearing Japanese from his mother and English from his father. (Photo: Janet Werker, UBC Infant Studies Centre)

Scientists, linguistics and neurologists mostly, have always been fascinated by how children effortlessly acquire the grammar of their native language even though speech provides no direct information about underlying structure. The idea becomes even more curious and fascinating when the children in question are exposed to two languages with great difference in grammar between them.

It’s rather remarkable how the infant mind can acquire seemingly difficult grammatical properties of a language, and the mechanics and means through which this is achieved is only beginning to be understood properly. Frequency and order of words play a significant role in the learning process, but the present findings suggest that  mean duration, mean pitch, maximum pitch and mean intensity of the stressed vowels of content words  and functors play a major as well.

Typically languages differ greatly in the basic word order type defined by the relative order of the verb (V) and its object (O). For their study, the researchers looked at infants growing up in bilingual environments where one of the language was VO (English, Italian or Spanish) where prepositions and articles typically precede nouns (to London, the house), and the other was an OV language (Japanese, Turkish or Basque), where the reverse applies.

“By as early as seven months, babies are sensitive to these differences and use these as cues to tell the languages apart,” says UBC psychologist Janet Werker, co-author of the study.

The researchers’ results show that infants in bilingual environments use pitch and duration cues to discriminate between languages – such as English and Japanese – with opposite word orders, besides word frequency which is often sufficient for monolingual infants.

“For example, in English the words ‘the’ and ‘with’ come up a lot more frequently than other words – they’re essentially learning by counting,” says Judit Gervain, a linguist at the Université Paris Descartes and co-author of the new study. “But babies growing up bilingual need more than that, so they develop new strategies that monolingual babies don’t necessarily need to use.”

“If you speak two languages at home, don’t be afraid, it’s not a zero-sum game,” says Werker. “Your baby is very equipped to keep these languages separate and they do so in remarkable ways.”

Findings were reported in the journal Nature Communications.

source: University of British Columbia

share Share

A Dutch 17-Year-Old Forgot His Native Language After Knee Surgery and Spoke Only English Even Though He Had Never Used It Outside School

He experienced foreign language syndrome for about 24 hours, and remembered every single detail of the incident even after recovery.

If you use ChatGPT a lot, this study has some concerning findings for you

So, umm, AI is not your friend — literally.

Bad microphone? The people on your call probably think less of you

As it turns out, a bad microphone may be standing between you and your next job.

Looks Matter Most on Dating Apps By Far. And That's True for Both Men and Women

If you're struggling on dating apps, it's not because of your bio.

Just 10 Minutes of Mindfulness a Day Can Boost Your Mental Health

Daily short mindfulness sessions significantly reduce depression and anxiety while encouraging healthier lifestyles.

Why Can't We Remember Our Lives as Babies? Our Earliest Memories May Still be There

New research suggests infants can form memories far earlier than previously thought, but where do they go?

Most Back Pain Treatments Don't Work—Here's What Actually Helps

Sometimes, the simplest solutions—moving more, staying strong, and managing stress—are the best we have.

Is AI Moderation a Useful Tool or Another Failed Social Media Fix?

A new study suggests that an optimized AI model could detect harmful social media comments with 87% accuracy.

Study shows "Pro Life" supporters sometimes care more about banning casual sex than sanctity of life

Some Pro Life advocates may actually be subconsciously more fixated on the lives of the parents.

Less Than 1% of Gun Owners Use Their Firearms for Self-Defense Each Year. But Many More Are Exposed to Gun Violence

The study suggests gun proliferation carries more risks than self-defense benefits.